1 Introduction

“G and M walked into the restaurant. I wondered whether they were a couple. Then they kissed, as if to answer my question.”

(Part of a dream report, from the author’s personal dream journal, undated)

In the present paper I aim to: (1) examine the dynamic alternation in dreams between verbal or abstract thought generation (usually experienced as thoughts belonging to the dreamer or dream ego), and visual imagery generation (usually experienced as the dream presented to the dreamer or happening to the dreamer, including persons, objects, and places experienced as distinct from the self). One such alternation is portrayed in the dream example above. (2) Next, I will relate this associative pattern to waking spontaneous thought by suggesting that some people are more prone to such a dreamlike waking cognitive style. (3) Finally, I aim to explore whether this framework for understanding patterns of internally generated spontaneous mental activity could contribute to discerning different types of psychopathologies, with a special focus on obsessive-compulsive symptoms, significant dissociative identity alterations, and a recently identified syndrome of compulsive fantasy addiction.

2 The origin of visual imagery in dreams: Dreams as continuous image-thought-image associations

Throughout history, dreams have often been viewed as a succession of images that together form a narrative or fanciful storyline. For most people, sensory dream elements are mostly in the visual modality (Snyder, 1970), not unlike the predominant perceptual mode in wakefulness (Posner et al., 1976). Henceforth, I will use the word “images” when referring to visual internal representations. There has been debate over whether this succession of oneiric images portrays a narrative, bears meaningful clinical significance, or even conveys an underlying message, on one hand, or rather whether it is random, epiphenomenal, or meaningless brain activation on the other hand. The former, narrative view is well represented in the classic writings of Freud (1900) and his successors as well as modern psychodynamic and other clinicians interpreting dreams with their clients, under the premise that the dream has a meaning and extracting it would be clinically useful (Gennaro et al., 2020; Hill et al., 2008; Schredl et al., 2000). In keeping with the impressions of clinicians working with dreams, there is some empirical evidence supporting the notion that dreamwork in therapy can facilitate self-understanding and adaptive outcomes (e.g., Hill & Goates, 2004). Recalling and working with dreams or utilizing various dream-related techniques has been suggested to contribute to personal growth, both by religions or cultural traditions (e.g., Savary et al., 1984) and through the lens of empirical and practical psychology (e.g., Falk & Hill, 1995; M. Schredl in Hobson & Schredl, 2011; Konkoly & Burke, 2019; Provost, 1999). There is evidence relating dreams to the processing of salient emotional concerns, suggesting that perhaps emotion is the mechanism guiding the incorporation of elements into dreams (e.g., Malinowski & Horton, 2014a; Scarpelli et al., 2019). Moreover, dreams have been likened to psychotherapy, as they seem to associatively bind novel emotional events with loosely related existing concepts and memories and hence weave an integrated sense of self (Hartmann, 1998). Conversely, the view ascribing randomness to dream images arose during the 1970’s primarily in Allan Hobson’s “answer” to Freud (Hobson & McCarley, 1977). The theory had since then been revised and broadened but the basic claim was maintained: the succession of images in dreams is a psychologically meaningless, arbitrary flow of random activations originating from the brainstem, which the cortex then tries to tie together to a semi-coherent narrative. In other words, Hobson and colleagues argued that dreams are primarily made up of low-level internally generated signals, making their way to the cortex and being processed and synthesized by higher-order areas (Hobson & Pace-Schott, 2002; Hobson et al., 2000).

The Freudian “underlying message” view and Hobson’s “random epiphenomenal” view could be seen as two extremes of a continuum suggesting different degrees of meaning in dream images and storylines. Where do most dreams lie on this continuum? Evidence accumulated over the years through scientific research suggests that dreams do seem to be related to current emotionally salient concerns or preoccupations and are continuous to some extent with the waking thought patterns characterizing the individual (Bulkeley & Domhoff, 2010; Domhoff, 2001, 2011a, 2017; Erdelyi, 2017; Koffel & Watson, 2009; Pesant & Zadra, 2006; Schredl, 2003; Sterpenich et al., 2007;   Sterpenich et al., 2009; Van Rijn et al., 2015).1 Preoccupations seem to affect dreaming especially in those who tend to suppress or avoid their emotions (Alfasi & Soffer-Dudek, 2018; Malinowski, 2015), lending support to the notion that dreaming is involved in processing emotion. Hobson himself fully agreed that dreams incorporate some experiences or memory traces from waking life, however, he downplayed their importance compared to continuity theorists, and stated that he was more interested in better understanding novel or discontinuous dream elements (Hobson & Schredl, 2011). Thus, the completely random extreme of the continuum would be difficult to support empirically.

However, the other end of the continuum, namely, the Freudian notion that dreams convey an underlying complex message, conflict, or idea (Freud, 1900), implies that the process of dream generation is one of creating a coherent story or that the dream is a whole, with a specific starting point and ending. Is the ending of a dream already planned while the dream is starting out? If so, who is the homunculus screenplay writer directing the scenes in such an integrated way?2 This idea is also probably unrealistic. Just as our waking thought may be seen as an endless stream of associations (James, 1890), so are our dreams spontaneously changing from moment to moment, each shift in experience guided by its predecessor. In that sense, dreams are perhaps the ultimate manifestation of spontaneous thought (Christoff et al., 2016), due to the reduced executive control and meta-cognition, manifested in prefrontal deactivation (e.g., Braun et al., 1997; Dang-Vu et al., 2007). Moreover, compared to waking thought, this moment-to-moment change is hyperassociative (Hartmann, 1996; Horton & Malinowski, 2015; Llewellyn, 2013; Malinowski & Horton, 2015) and tends more to blend or associate different modalities, similar to synesthesia (Reznik et al., 2018). Arguably, the two main alternating modalities in dreams are visual images (experienced as reminiscent of visual perception or “inner seeing”) on one hand, and verbal or unsymbolized abstract thoughts on the other hand. By verbal thoughts I refer to inner speaking or hearing, including the experience of specific words being present, spoken, or heard, and possibly voice and tone inflection, whereas unsymbolized thought does not include these elements (Hurlburt et al., 2013). However, Hurlburt and colleagues mention that many people do not naturally notice this distinction. Rather, they may quickly translate preverbal to verbal mental activity and pay attention only to the latter. For the purpose of the present work, the distinction between preverbal and verbal mental activity is inconsequential, as my focus is on imagery versus image-less thoughts (See Hurlburt et al., 2013 for a formal discussion of the properties of each type of inner experience; and Hurlburt et al., 2022, for their frequencies using different assessment methods). To conclude, for most people dreaming seems to be mostly the experience of seeing and verbally or abstractly thinking.3 This could also be construed as a division between the dream (that is not experienced as our own creation, i.e., is non-agentive), and the dreamer (experienced as our own thoughts and beliefs during the oneiric event, i.e., the agentive part of the dream), respectively.

3 Dispelling the purported hierarchy between visual and verbal thought: Images are also associatively activated

Abstract or verbal thoughts in dreams have usually been viewed as a higher-level response to dream images, which were viewed as a lower-level mental element. In other words, intuitive or old-school perceptions of dreaming have focused on the image-to-(abstract) thought direction of causality, inspired by bottom-up models of perception, likening dreaming to waking perception (Foulkes & Domhoff, 2014). This is exemplified in Hobson’s activation-synthesis model described above: low level activation flows from the brainstem creating random images which are then synthesized by higher “thinking” parts into a story of sorts (Hobson & Pace-Schott, 2002; Hobson & McCarley, 1977). The image-to-thought view is also strongly implicated in the notion that in (non-lucid) dreams, the dreamer simply reacts to dream events, with reduced volition, rather than shapes and directs them with reflective awareness (Rechtschaffen, 1978).

However, it has been argued that dreaming is more like imagination than perception (Nir & Tononi, 2010), and images are themselves a type of internally generated thought. They are not bottom-up memories either: exact episodic memory replay in dreaming is rare, and dream images are better construed as novel creations of the mind (Malinowski & Horton, 2014b; Schwartz, 2003). Hence, dreams are made up of continuously reciprocal moment-to-moment influences between visual images and verbal or abstract thoughts, meaning that such thoughts shape dream images, and not just vice versa. An example of an abstract thought shaping the dynamic flow of images in a non-lucid dream is presented above through a section of a dream from the author’s personal dream journal. This example is an introspective phenomenological self-report, which, despite important technological advances in the scientific study of dreams, is still central and even critical for studying dream consciousness (Sikka, 2019). The dream example portrays how the dreamer reacts to an image with a non-visual thought, which then, in turn, generates a new visual image. It is clear in this example that the image of the couple kissing was directly influenced by the thought that preceded it, but during the ongoing dream, it does not subjectively feel that way. This is because the thinking part of the dream is experienced with a sense of agency (I was wondering whether they were a couple), whereas the oneiric images feel like an external event rather than our own mind’s creation. Yet, images in dreams are meaningful thoughts or associations, even though they are non-agentive. In other words, nocturnal dreams seem to be made up of ever-changing image-thought-image momentary associations, which are probably influenced by the dreamer’s cognitive schemas, guiding associative mental activity in both sleep and waking. Accordingly, it has been previously theorized that the dream “ego” continuously reflects upon, and responds to, the dream imagery throughout the dream in a process of synchronous moment-to-moment co-creation (Sparrow, 2014).4 This also relates to previous works acknowledging that high-order cognitive processing is common in nonlucid dreams (Kahan & LaBerge, 2011; Kozmová & Wolman, 2006; Wolman & Kozmová, 2007), and that dream imagery is often representative of metaphorical, figurative thinking (Hall, 1953).

To conclude, the creation of meanings, words, and images are all part of a top-down process during dreaming (Cicogna & Bosinelli, 2001). Our minds not only respond verbally or semantically to visual stimuli, but also respond visually to semantic associations, together creating a visual-verbal associative, perhaps even iterative, trail. Notably, this succession of mutual influences from images to thoughts and vice versa may be affected or modulated by various higher-level and lower-level processes, such as the individual’s personality and cognitive characteristics (Hartmann & Kunzendorf, 2006), conceptions and personal concerns or preoccupations (Domhoff, 2017), motivational/reward system determining which memories to incorporate through emotional relevance (Perogamvros & Schwartz, 2012), and memory consolidation or reprocessing needs (Cicogna & Bosinelli, 2001).

Notably, it is convenient to use the term “top-down” to convey the idea that dream images are associative visual thoughts rather than sensory input, but in the last decade or two it has become increasingly acknowledged in the field of visual processing that the concepts of “bottom-up” and “top-down” are poorly defined and are perhaps overly simplistic, which may also apply here. Specifically, the process of seeing and interpreting the world is now considered to involve numerous levels and junctions of flowing information with various feed-forward and feedback influences (Rauschenberger, 2010; Rauss & Pourtois, 2013), and the same could be said about dream associations. The ongoing image-thought-image flow in dreams may be a primarily top-down process but at the same time involve both feedforward and feedback processes between images and non-visual thoughts. This view of dreams is compatible with current formulations in the field of visual processing, hypothesizing distributed, non-hierarchical processing in higher areas, as opposed to the hierarchical exchange of information postulated previously (Hegdé & Felleman, 2007; Rauss & Pourtois, 2013).

The notion that mental activity or the flow of associations in dreams is a combination of visual and non-visual thinking, both representing high-level processes, is supported by research showing that the subjective experience of a dream seems to stem largely from cortical activity. Specifically, cortical regions associated with imagination and thinking are activated when individuals experience dream imagery (Bréchet et al., 2020; Siclari et al., 2017) and dream thoughts (Perogamvros et al., 2017), respectively. Although bottom-up influences also modulate the development of a dream (Cicogna & Bosinelli, 2001), as can be inferred from subcortical activations (e.g., Bréchet et al., 2020), I maintain that the central associative process of the image-thought-image experience in dreaming is a higher, largely cortical event, attesting to the human ability to think pictorially.5

4 Predictive coding and dreaming: Are visual-verbal associations in dreams top-down expectations?

One perspective considered more nuanced than the top-down – bottom-up distinction is the predictive coding framework. Predictive coding theory pertains to the continuous updating of predictions from an internal mental model of the world following feedback based on errors in those predictions (Friston, 2018; Rao & Ballard, 1999). Predictive coding is considered a bidirectional process which involves both bottom-up and top-down streams of information (Friston, 2018). Hobson et al. (2014) suggest that the predictive coding framework can be applied to dreaming and that dreams may lay the foundations for waking perception by refining the model in a simple environment free of sensory constraints. Llewellyn further elaborated on this idea suggesting that dreams represent prospective coding, which is the offline process of creating the codes and expectations that would eventually enable waking predictive coding (Llewellyn, 2015).

Arguably, it could be said that the succession of thoughts and images in dreams is in essence our attentional spotlight. In dreaming we “see” and think about, only what we are attending to; objects outside our attention span simply do not exist. Predictions and expectations guide the associative visual-verbal trail, without the corresponding external feedback or learning from prediction errors that occur in waking. This associative trail is meaningful because it reveals our expectations and mental model of the world: showing the movement of attention in the absence of external interference. Thus, it could be argued once again that dreams follow the individual’s cognitive schema and represent high-level processes or pictorial, figurative thinking (Hall, 1953).

Notably, however, Hobson and colleagues claim that the hypoactivation of the prefrontal cortex that characterizes the dream state means that the dream is freed from “the top-down constraints of narratives and perspective-taking” (Hobson et al., 2014, p. 8), suggesting that in their view, dreaming is a lower-level process, perhaps not significantly affected by constraints relating to the “self”, as manifested in prefrontal self-reflective awareness. This brings us back to the unresolved debate outlined above, namely, the question of the extent to which our personality, waking thought patterns, and expectations affect the flow of associations (primarily images and thoughts) in our dreams.

Based on the vast literature relating both dream content and structural characteristics of dreaming to our waking emotions and concerns (e.g., Domhoff, 2011a; Malinowski et al., 2014; Malinowski & Horton, 2014a; Schredl, 2008; Schredl & Reinhard, 2010), level of distress and emotional arousal (Levin & Nielsen, 2009; Soffer-Dudek, 2017a), and thought patterns and personality (Hartmann & Kunzendorf, 2006; Koffel & Watson, 2009), it seems plausible to say that even if dreams do not tell a coherent story or convey a full message from start to end, the momentary associations of alternating images and thoughts have some psychological meaning and relate to who we are and how we see the world. That is probably why many people find dreams helpful or significant for self-understanding (Beaulieu-Prévost et al., 2009; Hill & Goates, 2004; Morewedge & Norton, 2009). The realization that not only do images lead to thoughts, but vice versa as well, as exemplified in the dream excerpt above, elucidates how neuroticism, distress, and negative emotions cascade into nightmares (Levin & Nielsen, 2009). The dreamer’s cognitions, personal associations, and expectations affect the dream images as they are being created in the ongoing oneiric scene.

Considering the oneiric image-thought-image never-ending dynamic as a primarily top-down or feed-forward process establishes it as part of our mental model or associative framework: the model or framework through which we perceive and think in waking as well. For example, the degree to which we tend to blend different thoughts in waking (thin boundaries) correlates with the degree to which our dreams blend different concepts, resulting in increased bizarreness and hyper-associativity (Hartmann & Kunzendorf, 2006; Kunzendorf et al., 1997). Additionally, people with a tendency for unusual waking perceptions and experiences, as manifested in dissociative experiences and schizotypy, also tend to have more unusual dreams (Koffel & Watson, 2009). Finally, having the experience of control in lucid dreams seems to relate to a sense of control in waking (Aviram & Soffer-Dudek, 2018; Blagrove & Hartnell, 2000; Patrick & Durndell, 2004; Soffer-Dudek et al., 2011). These findings focus on individual differences in the ways in which people perceive the world in both the sleep state and the wake state. Below, I will discuss one more feature of dreams, and then examine whether there may be individual differences in a tendency for dream-like waking thought.

5 Agentive and non-agentive control in lucid and non-lucid dreams

Our state of mind in prototypical dreams is commonly described as reactive and passive (e.g., Rechtschaffen, 1978). Conversely, lucid dreams, dreams in which the dreamer is aware of their conscious state while dreaming (LaBerge, 1985; Van Eeden, 1913), are sometimes characterized by an experience of controlling the dream events. Notably, many lucid dreams lack this possible accompanying feature, and dreamers retain only the awareness of dreaming without any associated sense of control (Aviram & Soffer-Dudek, 2018). Such a combination (i.e., reflective awareness of dreaming devoid of a sense of control) has been associated with psychopathological distress (Aviram & Soffer-Dudek, 2018; Harb et al., 2016). Interestingly, even when lucid control is achieved, the control does not seem to be perfect, but rather partial (Windt & Voss, 2018), with unexpected distractions, and subordinate to bizarre dream logic (e.g., see Schädlich et al., 2017). Importantly, the dream this paper started out with underscores that significant control of the dream exists in non-lucid dreams as well, although it may not necessarily be experienced as control. We might verbally or abstractly think of something, and it will magically visually appear in our dream; we might wish for something, and the dream may steer us in that direction; these are thought-to-image influences, and they may explain why some people have benefitted from training in lucid dream therapy even though they failed in achieving lucidity (Spoormaker et al., 2003; Spoormaker & Bout, 2006; Zadra & Pihl, 1997): they may have experienced increased control and positive outcomes following a change in waking thought, which in turn affected their dream thought, even if they did not experience lucid reflective awareness within their dreams. Our abstract thoughts conjure up images as an immediate association, giving us a certain (albeit limited) amount of control of dream images through our associative thinking patterns, even in the absence of lucid/reflective awareness (Sparrow, 2014).

I propose that what in fact characterizes controlled lucid dreams is a sense of agency in navigating the dream events, which could be termed agentive control. An individual experiencing agentive control in a dream may feel that they are navigating the trail of verbal-visual associations with much more guidance and executive control over their attentional spotlight. Indeed, lucid dreaming is associated with higher prefrontal activity (Voss et al., 2009), which is usually deactivated in dreams compared to waking (Bréchet et al., 2020; Dang-Vu et al., 2007). Agentive control is quite rare in dreams as it is missing both from prototypical nonlucid dreams and from some lucid dreams. Moreover, there can be a strong sense of lack of control or inability to alter dream images/events, both in non-lucid and in lucid dreams (the latter often referred to as lucid nightmares), and as mentioned before, this may be indicative of distress.

Although a sense of agency or agentive control over our thoughts and actions is much more available to us in waking than dreaming, waking sense of agency is also subjected to significant individual differences. People will differ in the extent to which their behavior is experienced as willed versus automatic, relating to the concept of dissociative “absorption and imaginative involvement,” characterized by a tendency to act automatically with reduced sense of agency (Bregman-Hai et al., 2020), alongside strong imaginative abilities (Bregman-Hai et al., 2018). Below, I will suggest that such individual differences may mark the existence of a dreamlike cognitive style of spontaneous waking thought, including a combination of both elaborate imaginative processing (image-thought-image associations) and reduced sense of agency. Indeed, some individuals may mix sleep-wake states more than others (Soffer-Dudek, 2017a). The waking cognitions of certain individuals may be more dreamlike (Kunzendorf et al., 1997; Van der Kloet et al., 2012) just as some people’s dreams may be more wake-like (e.g., regarding memory access, motor excitation, or emotional arousal, see Soffer-Dudek (2017a)). I suggest that people with a salient dreamlike thinking style tend to experience such an associative pattern in waking, where non-visual thoughts provoke novel images which then provoke further thoughts, and this pictorial associative trail may be experienced with little agency. I will now turn to discussing individual differences in the salience of waking visual thought.

6 Individual differences in visual, pictorial, or imagery-led thinking during wakefulness, with reduced agency: The dreamlike cognitive style

There are considerable individual differences in people’s ability or ease in conjuring up novel visual image associations, as can be inferred from the research literature on imagery (e.g., D’Argembeau & Linden, 2006; Iachini et al., 2019; Isaac & Marks, 1994), the tendency for imaginative involvement (e.g., Bregman-Hai et al., 2018), suggestibility (e.g., Crossman et al., 2004), and “fantasy-proneness” (e.g., Merckelbach et al., 2022). High vividness of imagery relates to an immersive experience or sense of presence, but these do not necessarily imply agency and control over one’s imagery, as the ability to control imagery is only weakly related to these constructs (Iachini et al., 2019). Research on mild dissociative experiences (“absorption and imaginative involvement”; Carlson & Putnam (1993)) may be especially relevant as it taps into a combination of acting automatically with reduced agency (Soffer-Dudek & Somer, 2022) and being prone to be led by one’s internal images, as a result confusing real events with those that were only imagined or dreamt (Soffer-Dudek, 2017b; see also Wamsley et al., 2014 for another account of such confusion). Absorption and imaginative involvement is a trait that assesses an inclination to become immersed in daydreaming and other internal and external stimuli to the point of obliviousness to one’s current surroundings, pointing to a state of mind characterized by a narrow attentional spotlight (Soffer-Dudek et al., 2015) with little meta-cognition, somewhat reminiscent of the “single-mindedness” of dreams (Rechtschaffen, 1978). The concept of absorption and imaginative involvement relates to common experiences that to some degree occur in most people, and low-to-moderate levels are not necessarily indicative of psychopathology, although high levels are associated with various types of psychopathologies (Soffer-Dudek et al., 2015). Regardless of psychopathology, in a selected healthy sample, participants who scored high on absorption and imaginative involvement seemed to have a distinct cognitive profile. Specifically, in most executive functioning and working memory tasks they had decreased accuracy, stemming from commission rather than omission errors; but on a mental rotation task, requiring mentally rotating a visual stimulus, they were much faster than their counterparts, with no significant loss of accuracy (Bregman-Hai et al., 2018). The authors concluded that these individuals probably experience images as more salient and vivid, and consequently can manipulate them with ease; yet their tendency for an elaborate visual processing style may also lead to confabulation and hence, inaccuracy in other working memory tasks.

The cognitive style of thinking in pictures or readily conjuring up novel images may be seen as a tendency for image-thought-image associations or more dreamlike thinking during waking. This is opposed to people who have less of an inclination towards waking imagery or pictorial thought, who tend to focus mainly on verbal or abstract semantic concepts during wakefulness. For example, people may be distraught by negative semantic self-evaluations or abstract self-derogatory and self-critical appraisals (e.g., “you are worthless”), predisposing them to depression (Shahar, 2015); conversely, others may be inclined to imagine themselves repeatedly being victimized or abused in different, novel ways (Somer, Abu-Rayya, et al., 2021). Both are indicative of maladaptive, self-berating associations, but one does not necessarily involve salient images, whereas the other is significantly affected by imagery. Such differing cognitive styles may predispose different individuals to specific psychopathologies and bear implications in terms of appropriate psychotherapy. In both, a sense of uncontrollability and automaticity is associated with psychopathological distress. In other words, waking spontaneous thought may be driven by negative affect, as seen in cognitive biases in anxiety and depression (e.g., Krantz & Hammen, 1979; Miranda & Mennin, 2007), regardless of whether the associations involve visual imagery. When the flow of associations is experienced as automatic, biased, or intrusive the individual may feel significant psychopathological distress. It has been suggested that more automaticity means more constraints, translating to less spontaneous or free-floating thought, as can be seen for example in rumination (Christoff et al., 2016). However, Christoff and colleagues did not differentiate image-based associations from imageless ones. I will argue that salience of imagery or the tendency to experience thought-image-thought chains in waking spontaneous cognition predisposes to specific psychopathologies.

7 From dreaming to waking: The dreamlike cognitive style as a framework for understanding types of spontaneous thought, and its relevance for psychopathology

With a paucity of external sensory input, dreams provide a unique opportunity to study the workings of the mind. In this paper, I focus on the image-thought-image two-way streak identified above to offer a potentially useful distinction of types of mind-wandering or spontaneous thought: those with salient imagery associations, versus those without.

Specifically, mind-wandering is an umbrella term referring to mental activity that is either experienced as unintentional or free-associating, or is unrelated to the task at hand, or is intrinsically generated (Seli et al., 2018). It has been suggested to represent a subtype of spontaneous-thought phenomena, i.e., thoughts that arise freely with weak deliberate constraints, alongside nocturnal dreaming and creative thinking (Christoff et al., 2016). Nocturnal dreaming and mind-wandering/daydreaming have all been associated with the default mode network and purportedly use the same simulation processes (Domhoff, 2011b). However, off-task thinking, mind-wandering, or spontaneous thought, are concepts that are loosely or broadly defined, that need to be better conceptualized (e.g., Christoff et al., 2016, 2018; Theodor-Katz et al., 2022). These concepts could be used to denote very different types or patterns of off-task mentation, including rumination, worry or free-floating anxiety, incoherent associative, distractable, and unguided thoughts, pondering the day’s to-do list, intrusive imagery, or even fanciful, alluring images or stories (Theodor-Katz et al., 2022).

Importantly, different types of associative mental activity play pivotal roles in the formation of different psychopathologies. A tendency for dreamlike thought, i.e., pictorial thinking and strong image-thought-image associations with reduced agentive control, as a driving force in our mental activity may be one potentially useful distinction. Such imagery-driven or dreamlike mind-wandering may predispose a distressed individual to certain types of maladaptive spontaneous cognitive patterns, namely, intrusive images or images that create or significantly interact with anxiety. Such imagery is associated to a great extent with obsessive-compulsive symptoms, dissociation, and excessive fantasy. The next sections will discuss the role of spontaneous novel imagery or image-thought-image associations in all three of these clinical conditions, all three of which are also theoretically or empirically associated with reduced sense of agency. Finally, I will present some concluding ideas about dreamlike spontaneous waking thought, including implications for interventions.

8 Dreamlike cognitions in obsessive-compulsive disorder

I suggest that the continuous succession of images to verbal or abstract thoughts and from such thoughts to images, as described above regarding dreaming, may be especially relevant to understanding the dynamics of obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD). Intrusive imagery is a central feature in OCD (Bouvard et al., 2017; Lipton et al., 2010; Rachman, 2007; Speckens et al., 2007). Here is one case example for how a distressing thought turns to an intrusive image in a patient with OCD:

Whenever I see or hear an aircraft overhead, I look at it initially I think, a nice sight, I see the trailers coming from the engines and then picture the people inside, that they might be getting ready to land, pilot speaking to them and then I think I hope it doesn’t blow up or something like that and I get pictures of the engines on fire, surrounded by giant orange flames and black smoke and then I visualise the whole plane blowing up.

(Taken from: Lipton et al., 2010, p. 820)

The thought “I hope it doesn’t blow up” is instantly translated to the associated images of an aviation disaster. Possibly, individuals prone to develop OCD may be those who tend to think visually or easily conjure up a novel image in response to an abstract idea (Soffer-Dudek, 2023). A tendency for an image-thought-image (or dreamlike) cognitive style may mean that their mental imagery is especially salient or vivid, rendering it harder to get rid of or control, i.e., more intrusive. Indeed, people with OCD have an impaired sense of agency (e.g., Oren et al., 2016, 2019). Interestingly, the memory associated with the intrusive image of the plane described above involves an experience of helplessness and reduced agency, as the patient describes their frequently getting arbitrarily punished as a child even though they did nothing wrong (Lipton et al., 2010). Intrusive imagery may lead to dissociative immersion and engulfment in the obsession. Accordingly, obsessive-compulsive symptoms are strongly associated with dissociation and especially with absorption and imaginative involvement (Soffer-Dudek, 2014, 2017b, 2019, 2023; Soffer-Dudek et al., 2015). A vivid, hyperactive, or easily triggered imagery system or an imagery-led cognitive style would also result in imagined possibilities being more easily confused with reality. This is known in the literature on OCD as the cognitive tendency for “inferential confusion” (Aardema et al., 2005; O’Connor & Robillard, 1995). Individuals high in inferential confusion tend to rely excessively on their imagination at the expense of trusting their sensory input, and this predisposes them to OCD (Aardema et al., 2005; Aardema & O’Connor, 2003; O’Connor & Robillard, 1995). In the case example above, the patient is concerned that their unsolicited thoughts will somehow put the plane in danger of actually blowing up (Lipton et al., 2010). As expected, inferential confusion is strongly associated with dissociative absorption and imaginative involvement, and both predict OCD with significant shared variance between them (Aardema & Wu, 2011; Pozza et al., 2016). Relatedly, in an exploration of the dream characteristics of people with obsessive-compulsive symptoms, an especially strong predictor was dream-reality confusion (Yu, 2013), possibly alluding to the salience and vividness of dream imagery or the impressions they leave behind.

Notably, pictorial thinking cannot include negation; thus, an abstract thought such as “I hope the plane doesn’t blow up” or “I would never want to have sex with another woman” would, paradoxically, conjure up an image of the negated event as if it were occurring (Soffer-Dudek, 2023). The patient depicted in Lipton et al. (2010) reports their resulting guilt at the prospect of the plane being physically harmed following their thoughts and images, and consider themselves to be “evil”, retroactively attaching malicious intent to oneself even though the conjured-up image was intrusive and unwanted. Similarly, an image of having sex with another woman would probably immediately raise guilt-ridden abstract thoughts or speculations such as “Perhaps I secretly do desire to have sex with other women?” Such a speculation would be especially likely in people with OCD as they tend to allocate exaggerated meaning to their spontaneous images and thoughts (Shafran & Rachman, 2004). Thus, the tendency to engage in thought-image-thought reciprocal associations may predispose individuals to OCD. Future studies should examine whether nocturnal dreams in OCD would be recalled as more vivid.

9 Dreamlike cognitions in dissociative identity disorder

A tendency for absorption and imaginative involvement is considered to represent dissociation (Carlson & Putnam, 1993; Soffer-Dudek & Somer, 2022) and is elevated in samples with dissociative identity disorder (DID) (Leavitt, 1999, 2001; Ross et al., 1995).6 DID is a disorder inherently characterized by a decreased sense of agency as one’s emotions, memories, thoughts, and behaviors are attributed to parts experienced as “not-me” or independently agentive (Soffer-Dudek & Somer, 2022). Imaginative involvement may play a pertinent role in the disorder as some individuals with DID present with a rich and complex inner world. Images and thoughts probably rapidly interchange as abstract thoughts and feelings are instantly metaphorically translated into the musings of visualized alternate agentive identities. This is exemplified in the following clinical excerpt from a therapy session with a person suffering from DID, who believes (or rather, most of her experienced parts believe) that she has endured satanic ritual abuse:

Therapist: What’s going on with those guys on the inside right now?

Patient: They think you’re trying to trick them. They’re telling me not to listen to you.

(Taken from: Ross, 2018, p. 169)

The patient, experiencing herself as a host for several agentive identities, continuously creates a complex inner space where metaphorical parts of her self produce dialogues and create a narrative. For example, in the excerpt presented, her probably preverbal or abstract thought according to which she should feel suspicion towards the therapist instantly translated into an internal scene where characters or parts with shared lived experience were imagined to be the source of inner speaking, conveying that thought. Although we cannot know for sure that this was experienced visually, many patients with DID report the visual properties of alternate identities or even inner locations, suggesting that at least for some, this dynamic includes a strong visual element. Thus, we may assume that the client experienced an associative chain whereby abstract thought led to visual imagery, which in turn led to verbal inner speaking. In a later part of the session, following a discussion about the death of the patient’s father, the visual properties of the client’s thought pattern are even more strongly implied through her description of a non-verbal inner event:

Therapist: What are the cult alters doing right now?

Patient: They’re crying.

Therapist: What are they crying about?

Patient: You said their father is dead.

(Taken from: Ross, 2018, p. 170)

Again, an abstract idea (father’s death, mentioned by the therapist), or perhaps an abstract feeling (sadness), seems to be translated into a probably visual experience of crying alters. DID is a disorder that involves self-hypnosis or auto-suggestion to a great extent (Pancheri et al., 2017), similar to conversion disorder (Oakley, 1999). In hypnosis, verbal suggestions “looking for” certain things inside one’s mind lead to the instantaneous creation of corresponding images, which then translate to novel abstract ideas or meanings. Again, this process is similar to the dreaming mind, where verbalized thoughts provoke images which in turn generate further thoughts, and this process is accompanied by reduced sense of agency. It is also reminiscent of the process of “knowing” in a dream what non-self dream characters are thinking or feeling. The dreamer projects their own ideas on an experienced, visualized separate agent. Indeed, as identified by Deirdre Barrett almost 30 years ago in an investigation of the dream characteristics of people with DID:

For most people, the state of dreaming is characterized by a degree of hallucination, amnesia, discontinuity with normal experience, and projection of aspects of themselves onto others that they experience at no other time. One of the most striking observations of this survey is that dissociative disordered persons experience much more of this dreamlike state in their waking life.(Barrett, 1996, p. 80)

Somewhat relatedly, patients with dissociative disorders tend to have clinically significant nightmares (Agargun et al., 2003), and various milder dissociative experiences have been related to several types of disturbed dreaming and unusual sleep experiences, pointing to possible shared mechanisms (Koffel & Watson, 2009; Soffer-Dudek, 2017a; Van der Kloet et al., 2012; Watson, 2001). Further study is needed on nocturnal dreaming characteristics in DID (e.g., vividness, bizarreness).

10 An imagery disorder: Maladaptive daydreaming

Maladaptive Daydreaming (MD) is a recently suggested syndrome or disorder, whereby the essence of the person’s problem is excessive, addictive immersion in vivid, intensely emotional, and narrative waking fantasy (Somer, 2002; Somer, Soffer-Dudek, Ross, & Halpern, 2017). During immersion in their fantasy activity, people with MD may laugh, cry, or become excited; they may feel intense love, sadness, or hate towards their imagined characters. Maladaptive daydreamers report a constant urge to engage in this activity, with varying degrees of agency and control versus intrusiveness. Specifically, some experience their fantasies as appearing spontaneously or automatically, especially if they are bored or distressed, and particularly if they experience triggers like evocative music, books or tv shows, or stereotypical movement (Bigelsen & Schupak, 2011). Stereotypical movements may include, for example, pacing, running, twirling, hand or finger movements, or rocking one’s body or limbs (Bigelsen et al., 2016; Jopp et al., 2019). These movements seem to aid daydreamers in inducing self-hypnosis by immersing themselves into a trance-like absorptive fantasy state (Somer, Cardeña, et al., 2021).

The following excerpt describing one maladaptive daydreamer’s experience exemplifies how excessive novel imagery has taken over her waking consciousness in an intrusive way, with reduced agency:

My imagination would come bursting in at the worst of times, for example during an exam or at the dinner table [...] in the form of dancing figures [which] would burst through the doors and windows of whatever room I was in, they would sing, spin and laugh around me, making jokes and encouraging me to join them. I always found them irritating. I wanted, needed to concentrate but couldn’t with all the racket they were causing. So, I would come up with imaginary scenarios to get rid of them (a black hole swallowing them up, a rainstorm washing them away in a flood, a dragon flying in and scaring them off), which of course drew me deeper into the world of fantasy […]

(Taken from: Soffer-Dudek & Somer, 2022, p. 549)

Others will actively and purposefully create the situations that may enable the fantasy activity, in a more premeditated way (e.g., turn down a social invitation so that they can daydream comfortably in the solitude of their home, purposefully go jogging with music on or close the door to their room and start to pace in circles). Often maladaptive daydreamers will report that they feel a growing urge to engage in their stories and inner world, until they cannot withstand it anymore and therefore submit to it, like an itch that needs to be scratched, or like any other behavioral addiction (Pietkiewicz et al., 2018; Somer, 2018). Importantly, not all individuals with the ability for immersive daydreaming necessarily suffer from maladaptive daydreaming (West & Somer, 2020). The defining feature of maladaptive daydreaming is its maladaptivity, defined like any other psychiatric syndrome according to the person’s own account of distress and interference with life goals and functioning (Soffer-Dudek et al., 2020).

Importantly, MD is different than rumination, obsessions, or post-traumatic flashbacks, in that the storylines are novel and unique in each daydreaming episode. In that sense, it is a very creative syndrome, involving spontaneous thought to a great extent. On the other hand, it is also very much constrained, both in the compulsive urge to engage in it, and in the underlying themes which are usually quite consistent, providing an overarching environment or context for the changing storylines, like a soap opera (Soffer-Dudek & Somer, 2018). Maladaptive Daydreamers who are trauma survivors may also tend to daydream repeatedly of trauma in different ways (not necessarily their actual trauma), either as victims, rescuers, or perpetrators in dramatic situations (Somer, Abu-Rayya, et al., 2021). For others, MD may revolve around different themes such as an ideal self, being cared for or soothed, or enacting sexual fantasies (Somer, 2002; Wen et al., 2022). Within these overarching themes, every daydream is narrative, creative and different; that is one of the characteristics that enable an MD-informed clinician to make a differential diagnosis between MD and obsessions, ruminations, or post-traumatic flashbacks, which are all characterized by being very fixed and constrained (Christoff et al., 2016). The following excerpt depicts the rich, elaborate, and novel nature of MD fantasies:

I have spun tons of plot lines in this world spanning multiple generations of characters [...] On the negative side, a character’s parents or best friend dies, a character is injured, abused, tortured or raped, or even just has a terrible argument with a loved one. On the positive side, a character reunites with a loved one he/she thought was dead, or realizes the person he/she has been in love with for a long time feels the same way, or finds out that she is going to have a baby. Characters fall in love, get married have and raise children, develop deep and strong friendships.

(Taken from: Bigelsen & Schupak, 2011, p. 1640)

The phenomenon of MD underscores the inadequacy of the simple distinction between spontaneous and goal-oriented thought. It suggests that we need more fine-grained conceptualizations. In the context of the present work, I hypothesize that the MD dynamic is probably similar to some extent to the workings of the dreaming mind, but with more prefrontal agentive control of the unfolding narrative. The imagery system or dreamlike cognitions of maladaptive daydreamers are probably more salient than they are in many other people. Perhaps, individuals who are engulfed in vivid images are inclined to create stories in their minds. In other words, images probably tend to solicit a unifying storyline: our mind constantly strives to bind the images into a narrative, not unlike the process of narrative making in nocturnal dreams. Indeed, people with high scores on MD reported higher levels of dream recall, emotional intensity of dreams, nightmare frequency, nightmare distress, recurring nightmares about daytime, lucid dreams, interest toward dreams, problem solving and creative dreams, and dreams affecting daytime mood (Margherita et al., 2023). No effect was observed regarding the emotional tone of dreams. Future research on nocturnal dreams of maladaptive daydreamers and on the neuropsychological correlates of MD may shed light on the similarities and differences between these two related fields.

11 Connection between the disorders supports a common cognitive style

As a final note, it is important to mention that there are significant inter-relations between the three psychopathological domains considered in this paper, namely, OCD, dissociation, and MD. Such inter-relations support the idea of a tendency for a certain cognitive style, manifested in a tendency for dreamlike, pictorial thinking, and reduced sense of agency. First, not surprisingly, MD is strongly related to dissociation with especially large effects for the dissociative subscale of absorption and imaginative involvement (Bigelsen et al., 2016; Ferrante et al., 2022; Jopp et al., 2019; Schimmenti et al., 2020; Soffer-Dudek & Somer, 2022; Somer et al., 2016). Moreover, MD is considered by several scholars to represent a dissociative disorder and to have important links with DID (Ross, 2018; Soffer-Dudek & Somer, 2022). In a sample of people with DID, there were extremely high rates of MD (Ross et al., 2020), and in a sample of people with MD, average dissociation scores were close to the clinical cutoff (Ferrante et al., 2022). In addition to the strong connection between MD and dissociation, there is also a strong connection between MD and OCD. Notably, MD is different than OCD in that it is gratifying in the short run, characterized by reward-seeking—similar to a behavioral addiction—rather than necessarily to threat avoidance. Nevertheless, astoundingly high rates of maladaptive daydreamers have comorbid obsessive-compulsive symptoms or OCD-related spectrum symptoms, including skin-picking, trichotillomania, and body dysmorphic disorder (Salomon-Small et al., 2021; Somer, Soffer-Dudek, & Ross, 2017). Interestingly, intrusive imagery is more salient in OCD with comorbid body dysmorphic disorder than in OCD alone (Nakata et al., 2007). In a sample of over 500 maladaptive daydreamers, approximately 56% of them were over the cutoff for suspected OCD, and dissociation was a strong mediating factor explaining the relationship of OCD and MD (Salomon-Small et al., 2021). Finally, OCD is significantly associated with dissociative experiences, and especially with absorption and imaginative involvement (Soffer-Dudek, 2014, 2017b, 2019, 2023; Soffer-Dudek et al., 2015). All these interconnections support the idea that some individuals may have a hyperactive imagery system, resulting in waking pictorial thinking and thought-image reciprocal associations, predisposing these individuals to certain types of psychopathologies. More studies are needed on the unique characteristics of nocturnal dreaming in these groups.

12 Conclusion

I have suggested that dreams are characterized by an ever-changing succession of image-thought-image reciprocal momentary hyper-associations, representing meaningful, “top-down” cognitive schemas of the dreamer. I argued that just as abstract or verbal thoughts in dreams are reactions to the dream images, images in dreams are similarly led by, or controlled by, the dreamer’s abstract thoughts, even if that control is not experienced as agentive. I have also maintained that there are individual differences in the extent to which such an oneiric thought dynamic may be present in waking spontaneous thought, which may benefit from more accurate definitions and conceptualizations.

What causes the individual tendency to think in a dreamlike or visual way? Further study is needed to understand whether these stable, trait-like individual differences are innate, or perhaps affected by one’s environment. For example, perhaps a parent may foster a child’s imagination by reading stories to them, or on a larger scale, perhaps a certain culture or language may nurture metaphorical thinking by using symbolic images in common idioms. However, state variables may also affect the nature of an individual’s mental activity. For example, sleep deprivation seems to lead to more dissociative experiences such as absorption and imaginative involvement, depersonalization, derealization and dreamlike thought (Giesbrecht et al., 2007; Selvi et al., 2015; Soffer-Dudek et al., 2017; Van der Kloet et al., 2012; Van Heughten – van der Kloet et al., 2015). More research is needed on the etiological factors explaining this type of cognitive style.

Finally, I suggested that this dreamlike dynamic may predispose individuals to certain psychopathologies. Importantly, this should not be taken to mean that such dreamlike thought is necessarily maladaptive; rather, this argument follows a diathesis-stress model, suggesting that each cognitive style, whether involving imagery or not, predisposes for certain types of psychopathologies in the face of certain stressors. It is also important to keep in mind that the imagery distinction suggested in this paper as a certain type of spontaneous thought is probably also an over-simplification and not specific enough for an accurate classification of people’s thought styles. Specifically, even if we focus on images as driving forces in one’s mental activity, we will notice the differences between the nature of the images and the nature of their power over the individual. For example, individuals may find themselves drawn to intrusive—or alluring—images, as may occur in OCD and MD, respectively. The act of succumbing to engaging with a tempting or interesting image is different from the attempt to repress a distressing image. It is also important to mention that the dreamlike cognitive style is not reserved only for the psychopathologies mentioned above. Examining the imagery salience of a client’s thought pattern is important in the context of other syndromes as well, to better classify their specific psychopathological profile, for example in depression or anxieties of different kinds, as well as psychosis.

Better understanding imagery-proneness in clinical conditions may bear implications for therapeutic interventions. For example, individuals with a dreamlike cognitive style may be better candidates for imaginal exposure interventions than those whose imaginations are less life-like. Moreover, it has been established that individuals who are high in absorption and imaginative involvement are less able to improve in their respective therapies for OCD, depression, and anxiety (e.g., Spitzer et al., 2007). Possibly, imagination-based interventions such as guided imagery or imagery rescripting/rehearsal (Arntz, 2012; Edwards, 2007; Krakow & Zadra, 2010; Smucker, 2005), or similarly, hypnotic memory restructuring (Gravitz, 1994), might prove to benefit them more than a purely verbal cognitive intervention. Indeed, it has been shown that imagery rescripting is useful in OCD, but knowledge is lacking regarding moderating factors that may interfere with, or facilitate, this efficacy (Strachan et al., 2020). In other words, baseline trait tendencies may possibly moderate the effect. Imagery rescripting has also been useful in the context of altering traumatic memories (Morina et al., 2017), and reductions in post-traumatic symptoms were associated with reductions in dissociation (Boterhoven de Haan et al., 2020). Notably, however, high dissociation tendencies have also been suggested as an exclusion criterion in imagery rescripting for trauma, for fear of uncontrolled dissociating during scripting (Smucker, 2005). Recent research suggests that in cases with high dissociation therapeutic gains with rescripting still exist but might be slower to achieve, because of detached responses to negative affect as well as loss of control over the image (Lechner-Meichsner et al., 2023; Paulik et al., 2022), pointing again to the importance of agency and control. Importantly, the implementation of grounding techniques, when necessary, may enable the dissociative client to safely utilize and benefit from imagery rescripting, which may be a powerful tool for them, by strengthening their awareness and control over their dissociative state (Lechner-Meichsner et al., 2023). Notably, imagery rescripting itself not only utilizes the client’s imaginative abilities, but also empowers clients’ sense of agency and control as they create their own novel, positive imagery. Various authors have underscored the importance that the imagery modification be the client’s personal (agentive) creation (e.g., Krakow & Zadra, 2010; Smucker, 2005). In conclusion, clinicians should ask patients not only about the thoughts that come to their mind, but also about the images that do so.

References

Aardema, F., & O’Connor, K. (2003). Seeing white bears that are not there: Inference processes in obsessions. Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy, 17(1), 23–37. https://doi.org/10.1891/jcop.17.1.23.58270
Aardema, F., O’Connor, K. P., Emmelkamp, P. M. G., Marchand, A., & Todorov, C. (2005). Inferential confusion in obsessive–compulsive disorder: The inferential confusion questionnaire. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 43(3), 293–308. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2004.02.003
Aardema, F., & Wu, K. D. (2011). Imaginative, dissociative, and schizotypal processes in obsessive‐compulsive symptoms. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 67(1), 74–81. https://doi.org/10.1002/jclp.20729
Agargun, M. Y., Kara, H., Özer, Ö. A., Selvi, Y., Kiran, Ü., & Özer, B. (2003). Clinical importance of nightmare disorder in patients with dissociative disorders. Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 57, 575–579. https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1440-1819.2003.01169.x
Alfasi, D., & Soffer-Dudek, N. (2018). Does alexithymia moderate the relation between stress and general sleep experiences? Personality and Individual Differences, 122, 87–92. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2017.10.014
Arntz, A. (2012). Imagery rescripting as a therapeutic technique: Review of clinical trials, basic studies, and research agenda. Journal of Experimental Psychopathology, 3(2), 189–208. https://doi.org/10.5127/jep.024211
Aviram, L., & Soffer-Dudek, N. (2018). Lucid dreaming: Intensity, but not frequency, is inversely related to psychopathology. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 384. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00384
Barrett, D. (Ed.). (1996). Dreams in multiple personality disorder. In Trauma and dreams (pp. 68–81). Harvard University Press.
Beaulieu-Prévost, D., Charneau Simard, C., & Zadra, A. (2009). Making sense of dream experiences: A multidimensional approach to beliefs about dreams. Dreaming, 19(3), 119–134. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0017279
Bigelsen, J., Lehrfeld, J. M., Jopp, D. S., & Somer, E. (2016). Maladaptive daydreaming: Evidence for an under-researched mental health disorder. Consciousness and Cognition, 42, 254–266. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2016.03.017
Bigelsen, J., & Schupak, C. (2011). Compulsive fantasy: Proposed evidence of an under-reported syndrome through a systematic study of 90 self-identified non-normative fantasizers. Consciousness and Cognition, 20, 1634–1648. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2011.08.013
Blagrove, M. (2011). Distinguishing continuity/discontinuity, function, and insight when investigating dream content. International Journal of Dream Research, 4(2), 45–47. https://doi.org/10.11588/ijodr.2011.2.9153
Blagrove, M., & Hartnell, S. J. (2000). Lucid dreaming: Associations with internal locus of control, need for cognition and creativity. Personality and Individual Differences, 28(1), 41–47. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0191-8869(99)00078-1
Boterhoven de Haan, K., Lee, C., Fassbinder, E., Van Es, S., Menninga, S., Meewisse, M., Rijkeboer, M., Kousemaker, M., & Arntz, A. (2020). Imagery rescripting and eye movement desensitisation and reprocessing as treatment for adults with post-traumatic stress disorder from childhood trauma: Randomised clinical trial. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 217(5), 609–615. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.2020.158
Bouvard, M., Fournet, N., Denis, A., Sixdenier, A., & Clark, D. (2017). Intrusive thoughts in patients with obsessive compulsive disorder and non-clinical participants: A comparison using the International Intrusive Thought Interview Schedule. Cognitive Behaviour Therapy, 46(4), 287–299. https://doi.org/10.1080/16506073.2016.1262894
Braun, A. R., Balkin, T. J., Wesenten, N. J., Carson, R. E., Varga, M., Baldwin, P., Selbie, S., Belenky, G., & Herscovitch, P. (1997). Regional cerebral blood flow throughout the sleep-wake cycle: An H2(15)o PET study. Brain, 120(7), 1173–1197. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/120.7.1173
Bréchet, L., Brunet, D., Perogamvros, L., Tononi, G., & Michel, C. M. (2020). EEG microstates of dreams. Scientific Reports, 10, 17069. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-74075-z
Bregman-Hai, N., Abitbul-Gordon, S., Deutsch, I., Garbi, D., Shelef, L., & Soffer-Dudek, N. (2018). Leave everything to the imagination: Cognitive functioning of individuals high in dissociative absorption and imaginative involvement. Journal of Research in Personality, 76, 129–138. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2018.08.004
Bregman-Hai, N., Kessler, Y., & Soffer-Dudek, N. (2020). Who wrote that? Automaticity and reduced sense of agency in individuals prone to dissociative absorption. Consciousness & Cognition, 78, 102861. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2019.102861
Bulkeley, K., & Domhoff, G. W. (2010). Detecting meaning in dream reports: An extension of a word search approach. Dreaming, 20(2), 77–95. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0019773
Carlson, E. B., & Putnam, F. W. (1993). An update on the dissociative experiences scale. Dissociation: Progress in the Dissociative Disorders, 6(1), 16–27. https://psycnet.apa.org/record/1994-27927-001
Christoff, K., Irving, Z. C., Fox, K. C. R., Spreng, R. N., & Andrews-Hanna, J. R. (2016). Mind-wandering as spontaneous thought: A dynamic framework. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 17, 718–731. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn.2016.113
Christoff, K., Mills, C., Andrews-Hanna, J. R., Irving, Z. C., Thompson, E., Fox, K. C. R., & Kam, J. W. Y. (2018). Mind-wandering as a scientific concept: Cutting through the definitional haze. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 22(11), 957–959. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2018.07.004
Cicogna, P., & Bosinelli, M. (2001). Consciousness during dreams. Consciousness and Cognition, 10(1), 26–41. https://doi.org/10.1006/ccog.2000.0471
Crossman, A. M., Scullin, M. H., & Melnyk, L. (2004). Individual and developmental differences in suggestibility. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 18(8), 941–945. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.1079
D’Argembeau, A., & Linden, M. van der. (2006). Individual differences in the phenomenology of mental time travel: The effect of vivid visual imagery and emotion regulation strategies. Consciousness and Cognition, 15(2), 342–350. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2005.09.001
Dang-Vu, T. T., Desseilles, M., Petit, D., Mazza, S., Montplaisir, J., & Maquet, P. (2007). Neuroimaging in sleep medicine. Sleep Medicine, 8(4), 349–372. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.sleep.2007.03.006
Dennett, D. C. (1976). Are dreams experiences? The Philosophical Review, 85, 151–171. https://doi.org/10.2307/2183728
Dentico, D., Cheung, B. L., Chang, J. Y., Guokas, J., Boly, M., Tononi, G., & Van Veen, B. (2014). Reversal of cortical information flow during visual imagery as compared to visual perception. Neuroimage, 100, 237–243. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.05.081
Domhoff, G. W. (2001). A new neurocognitive theory of dreams. Dreaming, 11(1), 13–33. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1009464416649
Domhoff, G. W. (2011a). Dreams are embodied simulations that dramatize conceptions and concerns: The continuity hypothesis in empirical, theoretical, and historical context. International Journal of Dream Research, 4(2), 50–62. https://doi.org/10.11588/ijodr.2011.2.9137
Domhoff, G. W. (2011b). The neural substrate for dreaming: Is it a subsystem of the default network? Consciousness and Cognition, 20(4), 1163–1174. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2011.03.001
Domhoff, G. W. (2017). The invasion of the concept snatchers: The origins, distortions, and future of the continuity hypothesis. Dreaming, 27(1), 14–39. https://doi.org/10.1037/drm0000047
Edwards, D. J. A. (2007). Restructuring implicational meaning through memory based imagery: Some historical notes. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 38, 306–316. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbtep.2007.10.001
Erdelyi, M. H. (2017). The continuity hypothesis. Dreaming, 27(4), 334–344. https://doi.org/10.1037/drm0000063
Falk, D. R., & Hill, C. E. (1995). The effectiveness of dream interpretation groups for women undergoing a divorce transition. Dreaming, 5(1), 29–42. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0094421
Ferrante, E., Marino, A., Guglielmucci, F., & Schimmenti, A. (2022). The mediating role of dissociation and shame in the relationship between emotional trauma and maladaptive daydreaming. Psychology of Consciousness: Theory, Research, and Practice, 9(1), 27–39. https://doi.org/10.1037/cns0000253
Foulkes, D., & Domhoff, G. W. (2014). Bottom-up or top-down in dream neuroscience? A top-down critique of two bottom-up studies. Consciousness & Cognition, 27, 168–171. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2014.05.002
Freud, S. (1900). Die traumdeutung. Franz Deuticke.
Friston, K. (2018). Does predictive coding have a future? Nature Neuroscience, 21, 1019–1021. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41593-018-0200-7
Gennaro, A., Kipp, S., Viol, K., Felice, G., Andreassi, S., Aichhorn, W., S, S., & Schiepek, G. (2020). A phase transition of the unconscious: Automated text analysis of dreams in psychoanalytic psychotherapy. Frontiers in Psychology, 11. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01667
Giesbrecht, T., Smeets, T., Leppink, J., Jelicic, M., & Merckelbach, H. (2007). Acute dissociation after 1 night of sleep loss. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 116(3), 599–606. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.116.3.599
Gravitz, M. A. (1994). Memory reconstruction by hypnosis as a therapeutic technique. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research, Practice, Training, 31(4), 687–691. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-3204.31.4.687
Hall, C. (1953). A cognitive theory of dream symbols. Journal of General Psychology, 48, 169–186. https://doi.org/10.1080/00221309.1953.9920189
Harb, G. C., Brownlow, J. A., & Ross, R. J. (2016). Posttraumatic nightmares and imagery rehearsal: The possible role of lucid dreaming. Dreaming, 26, 238–249. https://doi.org/10.1037/drm0000030
Hartmann, E. (1996). Outline for a theory on the nature and functions of dreaming. Dreaming, 6, 147–170. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0094452
Hartmann, E. (1998). Nightmare after trauma as paradigm for all dreams: A new approach to the nature and functions of dreaming. Psychiatry, 61(3), 223–238. https://doi.org/10.1080/00332747.1998.11024834
Hartmann, E., & Kunzendorf, R. G. (2006). Boundaries and dreams. Imagination, Cognition and Personality, 26(1), 101–115. https://doi.org/10.2190/HK76-038K-407M-8670
Hegdé, J., & Felleman, D. J. (2007). Reappraising the functional implications of the primate visual anatomical hierarchy. Neuroscientist, 13(5), 416–421. https://doi.org/10.1177/1073858407305201
Hill, C. E., & Goates, M. K. (2004). Research on the hill cognitive-experiential dream model. In C. E. Hill (Ed.), Dream work in therapy: Facilitating exploration, insight, and action (pp. 245–288). American Psychological Association.
Hill, C. E., Liu, J., Spangler, P., Sim, W., & Schottenbauer, M. (2008). Working with dreams in psychotherapy: What do psychoanalytic therapists report that they do? Psychoanalytic Psychology, 25(4), 565–573. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0013539
Hobson, A. J., & Pace-Schott, E. F. (2002). The cognitive neuroscience of sleep: Neuronal systems, consciousness and learning. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 3, 679–693. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn915
Hobson, J. A., Hong, C. C.-H., & Friston, K. J. (2014). Virtual reality and consciousness inference in dreaming. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1133. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01133
Hobson, J. A., & McCarley, R. W. (1977). The brain as a dream state generator: An activation-synthesis hypothesis of the dream process. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 134(12), 1335–1348. https://doi.org/10.1176/ajp.134.12.1335
Hobson, J. A., Pace-Schott, F. E., & Stickgold, R. (2000). Dreaming and the brain: Toward a cognitive neuroscience of conscious states. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 23, 793–842. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X00003976
Hobson, J. A., & Schredl, M. (2011). The continuity and discontinuity between waking and dreaming: A dialogue between michael schredl and allan hobson concerning the adequacy and completeness of these notions. International Journal of Dream Research, 4(1), 3–7. https://doi.org/10.11588/ijodr.2011.1.9087
Horton, C. L., & Malinowski, J. E. (2015). Autobiographical memory and hyperassociativity in the dreaming brain: Implications for memory consolidation in sleep. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 874. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00874
Hurlburt, R. T., Heavey, C. L., & Kelsey, J. M. (2013). Toward a phenomenology of inner speaking. Consciousness and Cognition, 22(4), 1477–1494. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2013.10.003
Hurlburt, R. T., Heavey, C. L., Lapping-Carr, L., Krumm, A. E., Moynihan, S. A., Kaneshiro, C., Brouwers, V. P., Turner, D. K., & Kelsey, J. M. (2022). Measuring the frequency of inner-experience characteristics. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 17(2), 559–571. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691621990379
Iachini, T., Maffei, L., Masullo, M., Senese, V. P., Rapuano, M., Pascale, A., Sorrentino, F., & Ruggiero, G. (2019). The experience of virtual reality: Are individual differences in mental imagery associated with sense of presence? Cognitive Processing, 20, 291–298. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-018-0897-y
Isaac, A. R., & Marks, D. F. (1994). Individual differences in mental imagery experience: Developmental changes and specialization. British Journal of Psychology, 85(4), 479–500. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8295.1994.tb02536.x
James, W. (1890). The principles of psychology. Macmillan & Co.
Jopp, D. S., Dupuis, M., Somer, E., Hagani, N., & Herscu, O. (2019). Validation of the hebrew version of the maladaptive daydreaming scale (MDS-h): Evidence for a generalizable measure of pathological daydreaming. Psychology of Consciousness: Theory, Research, and Practice, 6(3), 242–261. https://doi.org/10.1037/cns0000162
Kahan, T. L., & LaBerge, S. P. (2011). Dreaming and waking: Similarities and differences revisited. Consciousness and Cognition, 20, 494–514. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2010.09.002
Koffel, E., & Watson, D. (2009). Unusual sleep experiences, dissociation, and schizotypy: Evidence for a common domain. Clinical Psychology Review, 29(6), 548–559. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2009.06.004
Konkoly, K., & Burke, C. T. (2019). Can learning to lucid dream promote personal growth? Dreaming, 29(2), 113–126. https://doi.org/10.1037/drm0000101
Kozmová, M., & Wolman, R. N. (2006). Self-awareness in dreaming. Dreaming, 16(3), 196–214. https://doi.org/10.1037/1053-0797.16.3.196
Krakow, B., & Zadra, A. (2010). Imagery rehearsal therapy: Principles and practice. Sleep Medicine Clinics, 5(2), 289–298. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jsmc.2010.01.004
Krantz, S., & Hammen, C. L. (1979). Assessment of cognitive bias in depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 88(6), 611–619. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.88.6.611
Kunzendorf, R. G., Hartmann, E., Cohen, R., & Cutler, J. (1997). Bizarreness of the dreams and daydreams reported by individuals with thin and thick boundaries. Dreaming, 7(4), 265–271. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0094482
LaBerge, S. P. (1985). Lucid dreaming. Tarcher.
Leavitt, F. (1999). Dissociative experiences scale taxon and measurement of dissociative pathology: Does the taxon add to an understanding of dissociation and its associated pathologies? Journal of Clinical Psychology in Medical Settings, 6, 427–440. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1026275916184
Leavitt, F. (2001). MMPI profile characteristics of women with varying levels of normal dissociation. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 57(12), 1469–1477. https://doi.org/10.1002/jclp.1110
Lechner-Meichsner, F., Ehring, T., Kruger-Gottschalk, A., Morina, N., Plankl, C., & Steil, R. (2023). Using imagery rescripting to treat posttraumatic stress disorder in refugees: A case study. Cognitive and Behavioral Practice, 30(4), 646–656. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cbpra.2022.06.002
Levin, R., & Nielsen, T. (2009). Nightmares, bad dreams, and emotion dysregulation: A review and new neurocognitive model of dreaming. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 18(2), 84–88. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8721.2009.01614.x
Lipton, M. G., Brewin, C. R., Linke, S., & Halperin, J. (2010). Distinguishing features of intrusive images in obsessive–compulsive disorder. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 24(8), 816–822. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2010.06.003
Llewellyn, S. (2013). Such stuff as dreams are made on? Elaborative encoding, the ancient art of memory, and the hippocampus. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36(6), 589–607. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X12003135
Llewellyn, S. (2015). Dream to predict? REM dreaming as prospective coding. Frontiers in Psychology, 6. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01961
Malinowski, J. E. (2015). Dreaming and personality: Wake-dream continuity, thought suppression, and the big five inventory. Consciousness and Cognition, 38, 9–15. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2015.10.004
Malinowski, J. E., Fylan, F., & Horton, C. L. (2014). Experiencing “continuity”: A qualitative investigation of waking life in dreams. Dreaming, 24(3), 161–175. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0037305
Malinowski, J. E., & Horton, C. L. (2014a). Evidence for the preferential incorporation of emotional waking-life experiences into dreams. Dreaming, 24(1), 18–31. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0036017
Malinowski, J. E., & Horton, C. L. (2014b). Memory sources of dreams: The incorporation of autobiographical rather than episodic experiences. Journal of Sleep Research, 23(4), 441–447. https://doi.org/10.1111/jsr.12134
Malinowski, J. E., & Horton, C. L. (2015). Metaphor and hyperassociativity: The imagination mechanisms behind emotion assimilation in sleep and dreaming. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1132. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01132
Margherita, G., Caffieri, A., Mariani, R., Filosa, M., Manari, T., Lenzo, V., Quattropani, M. C., Vegni, E., Borghi, L., Castelnuovo, G., Saita, E., Freda, M. F., Varallo, G., Franceschini, C., & Musetti, A. (2023). Dreaming or daydreaming during COVID-19 lockdown: A comparison between maladaptive and nonmaladaptive daydreamers. Psychology of Consciousness: Theory, Research, and Practice, 10(4), 331–345. https://doi.org/10.1037/cns0000333
Merckelbach, H., Otgaar, H., & Lynn, S. J. (2022). Empirical research on fantasy proneness and its correlates 2000–2018: A meta-analysis. Psychology of Consciousness: Theory, Research, and Practice, 9(1), 2–26. https://doi.org/10.1037/cns0000272
Miranda, R., & Mennin, D. S. (2007). Depression, generalized anxiety disorder, and certainty in pessimistic predictions about the future. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 31, 71–82. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-006-9063-4
Morewedge, C. K., & Norton, M. I. (2009). When dreaming is believing: The (motivated) interpretation of dreams. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96(2), 249–264. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0013264
Morina, N., Lancee, J., & Arntz, A. (2017). Imagery rescripting as a clinical intervention for aversive memories: A meta-analysis. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 55, 6–15. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbtep.2016.11.003
Nakata, A. C. G., Diniz, J. B., Torres, A. R., Mathis, M. A., Fossaluza, V., & Braganças, C. A. (2007). Level of insight and clinical features of obsessive-compulsive disorder with and without body dysmorphic disorder. CNS Spectrums, 12(4), 295–303. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1092852900021052
Nielsen, T. (2017). Microdream neurophenomenology. Neuroscience of Consciousness, 2017(1), 1–17. https://doi.org/10.1093/nc/nix001
Nir, Y., & Tononi, G. (2010). Dreaming and the brain: From phenomenology to neurophysiology. Trends in Cognitive Science, 14(2), 88–100. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2009.12.001
Noreika, V., Canales-Johnson, A., Koh, J., Taylor, M., Massey, I., & Bekinschtein, T. A. (2015). Intrusions of a drowsy mind: Neural markers of phenomenological unpredictability. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 202. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00202
O’Connor, K. P., & Robillard, S. (1995). Inference processes in obsessive-compulsive disorder: Some clinical observations. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 33(8), 887–896. https://doi.org/10.1016/0005-7967(95)00042-V
Oakley, D. A. (1999). Hypnosis and conversion hysteria: A unifying model. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 4(3), 243–265. https://doi.org/10.1080/135468099395954
Oren, E., Eitam, B., & Dar, R. (2019). Intentional binding and obsessive-compulsive tendencies: A dissociation between indirect and direct measures of the sense of agency. Journal of Obsessive Compulsive and Related Disorders, 20, 59–65. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jocrd.2017.11.002
Oren, E., Friedmann, N., & Dar, R. (2016). Things happen: Individuals with high obsessive–compulsive tendencies omit agency in their spoken language. Consciousness and Cognition, 42, 125–134. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2016.03.012
Pancheri, C., Caredda, M., Pacchiarotti, I., Chiaie, R. D., & Biondi, M. (2017). Dissociative identity disorder, self-hypnosis and psychotic-like symptoms: A problem of differential diagnosis. Rivista Di Psichiatria, 52(5), 208–211. https://psycnet.apa.org/record/2017-54115-005
Patrick, A., & Durndell, A. (2004). Lucid dreaming and personality: A replication. Dreaming, 14(4), 234–239. https://doi.org/10.1037/1053-0797.14.4.234
Paulik, G., Newman-Taylor, K., Steel, C., & Arntz, A. (2022). Managing dissociation in imagery rescripting for voice hearers with trauma: Lessons from a case series. Cognitive and Behavioral Practice, 29(2), 434–445. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cbpra.2020.06.009
Perogamvros, L., Baird, B., Seibold, M., Riedner, B., Boly, M., & Tononi, G. (2017). The phenomenal contents and neural correlates of spontaneous thoughts across wakefulness, NREM sleep, and REM sleep. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 29(10), 1766–1777. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_01155
Perogamvros, L., & Schwartz, S. (2012). The roles of the reward system in sleep and dreaming. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 36(8), 1934–1951. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.05.010
Pesant, N., & Zadra, A. (2006). Dream content and psychological well-being: A longitudinal study of the continuity hypothesis. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 62(1), 111–121. https://doi.org/10.1002/jclp.20212
Pietkiewicz, I. J., Nęcki, S., Bańbura, A., & Tomalski, R. (2018). Maladaptive daydreaming as a new form of behavioral addiction. Journal of Behavioural Addictions, 7(3), 838–843. https://doi.org/10.1556/2006.7.2018.95
Posner, M. I., Nissen, M. J., & Klein, R. M. (1976). Visual dominance: An information-processing account of its origins and significance. Psychological Review, 83(2), 157–171. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.83.2.157
Pozza, A., Torniai, S., & Dèttore, D. (2016). Inferential confusion moderates the effects of dissociative experiences on OCD symptoms severity in a clinical sample with obsessive-compulsive disorder. Clinical Neuropsychiatry, 13(6), 108–111.
Provost, J. (1999). A dream focus for short-term growth groups. Journal for Specialists in Group Work, 24(1), 74–87. https://doi.org/10.1080/01933929908411420
Rachman, S. (2007). Unwanted intrusive images in obsessive compulsive disorders. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 38(4), 402–410. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbtep.2007.10.008
Rao, R., & Ballard, D. (1999). Predictive coding in the visual cortex: A functional interpretation of some extra-classical receptive-field effects. Nature Neuroscience, 2, 79–87. https://doi.org/10.1038/4580
Rauschenberger, R. (2010). Reentrant processing in attentional guidance – time to abandon old dichotomies. Acta Psychologica, 135(2), 109–111. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2010.04.014
Rauss, K., & Pourtois, G. (2013). What is bottom-up and what is top-down in predictive coding? Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 276. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00276
Rechtschaffen, A. (1978). The single-mindedness and isolation of dreams. Sleep, 1, 97–109. https://doi.org/10.1093/sleep/1.1.97
Reznik, D., Gertner-Saad, L., Even-Furst, H., Henik, A., Ben Mair, E., Shechter-Amir, D., & Soffer-Dudek, N. (2018). Oneiric synesthesia: Preliminary evidence for the occurrence of synesthetic-like experiences during sleep-inertia. Psychology of Consciousness: Theory, Research, and Practice, 5(4), 374–383. https://doi.org/10.1037/cns0000160
Ross, C. A. (2018). The potential relevance of maladaptive daydreaming in treatment of dissociative identity disorder in persons with ritual abuse and complex inner worlds. Frontiers in the Psychotherapy of Trauma and Dissociation, 1(2), 160–173. https://doi.org/10.XXXX/ftpd.2017.0010
Ross, C. A., Ellason, J. W., & Anderson, G. (1995). A factor analysis of the dissociative experiences scale (DES) in dissociative identity disorder. Dissociation, 8, 229–235. https://psycnet.apa.org/record/1996-00775-004
Ross, C. A., Ridgway, J., & George, N. (2020). Maladaptive daydreaming, dissociation, and the dissociative disorders. Psychiatric Research & Clinical Practice, 2(2), 53–61. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.prcp.20190050
Salomon-Small, G., Somer, E., Harel-Schwarzmann, M., & Soffer-Dudek, N. (2021). Maladaptive daydreaming and obsessive-compulsive symptoms: A confirmatory and exploratory investigation of shared mechanisms. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 136, 343–350. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2021.02.017
Savary, L. M. B., H., P., & Williams, S. K. (1984). Dreams and spiritual growth: A judeo-christian way of dreamwork. Paulist Press.
Scarpelli, S., Bartolacci, C., D’Atri, A., M, G., & Gennaro, L. (2019). The functional role of dreaming in emotional processes. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 459. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00459
Schädlich, M., Erlacher, D., & Schredl, M. (2017). Improvement of darts performance following lucid dream practice depends on the number of distractions while rehearsing within the dream – a sleep laboratory pilot study. Journal of Sports Sciences, 35(23), 2365–2372. https://doi.org/10.1080/02640414.2016.1267387
Schimmenti, A., Sideli, L., Marca, L., Gori, A., & Terrone, G. (2020). Reliability, validity, and factor structure of the maladaptive daydreaming scale (MDS–16) in an italian sample. Journal of Personality Assessment, 102(5), 689–701. https://doi.org/10.1080/00223891.2019.1594240
Schredl, M. (2003). Continuity between waking and dreaming: A proposal for a mathematical model. Sleep and Hypnosis, 5(1), 26–39. https://psycnet.apa.org/record/2003-03507-005
Schredl, M. (2008). Laboratory references in dreams: Methodological problem and/or evidence for the continuity hypothesis of dreaming? International Journal of Dream Research, 1(1), 3–6. https://doi.org/10.11588/heidok.00008425
Schredl, M., Bohusch, C., Kahl, J., Mader, A., & Somesan, A. (2000). The use of dreams in psychotherapy: A survey of psychotherapists in private practice. The Journal of Psychotherapy Practice and Research, 9(2), 81–87. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/10793127/
Schredl, M., & Reinhard, I. (2010). The continuity between waking mood and dream emotions: Direct and second-order effects. Imagination, Cognition and Personality, 29(3), 271–282. https://doi.org/10.2190/IC.29.3.f
Schwartz, S. (2003). Are life episodes replayed during dreaming? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(8), 325–327. https://doi.org/10.1016/s1364-6613(03)00162-1
Seli, P., Kane, M. J., Smallwood, J., Schacter, D. L., Maillet, D., Schooler, J. W., & Smilek, D. (2018). Mind-wandering as a natural kind: A family-resemblances view. Trends in Cognitive Science, 22(6), 479–490. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2018.03.010
Selvi, Y., Kiliç, S., Aydin, A., & Güzel Özdemir, P. (2015). The effects of sleep deprivation on dissociation and profiles of mood, and its association with biochemical changes. Archives of Neuropsychiatry, 52(1), 83–88. https://doi.org/10.5152/npa.2015.7116
Shafran, R., & Rachman, S. (2004). Thought-action fusion: A review. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 35(2), 87–107. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbtep.2004.04.002
Shahar, G. (2015). Erosion: The psychopathology of self-criticism. Oxford University Press.
Siclari, F., Baird, B., Perogamvros, L., Bernardi, G., LaRocque, J. L., Riedner, B., Boly, M., Postle, B. R., & Tononi, G. (2017). The neural correlates of dreaming. Nature Neuroscience, 20, 872–878. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.4545
Sikka, P. (2019). How to study dream experiences. In K. Valli & R. J. Hoss (Eds.), Dreams: Understanding biology, psychology, and culture (Vol. 1, pp. 153–166). Greenwood.
Smucker, M. R. (2005). Imagery rescripting and reprocessing therapy. In A. Freeman, S. H. Felgoise, C. M. Nezu, A. M. Nezu, & M. A. Reinecke (Eds.), Encyclopedia of cognitive behavior therapy (pp. 226–229). Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/0-306-48581-8_63
Snyder, F. (1970). The phenomenology of dreaming. In L. Madow & L. H. Snow (Eds.), The psychodynamic implications of the physiological studies on dreams (pp. 124–151). C. Thomas.
Soffer-Dudek, N. (2014). Dissociation and dissociative mechanisms in panic disorder, obsessive–compulsive disorder, and depression: A review and heuristic framework. Psychology of Consciousness: Theory, Research, and Practice, 1, 243–270. https://doi.org/10.1037/cns0000023
Soffer-Dudek, N. (2017a). Arousal in nocturnal consciousness: How dream- and sleep- experiences may inform us of poor sleep quality, stress, and psychopathology. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 733. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00733
Soffer-Dudek, N. (2017b). Daily elevations in dissociative absorption and depersonalization in a non-clinical sample are related to daily stress and psychopathological symptoms. Psychiatry-Interpersonal and Biological Processes, 80(3), 265–278. https://doi.org/10.1080/00332747.2016.1247622
Soffer-Dudek, N. (2019). Dissociative absorption, mind-wandering, and attention deficit symptoms: Associations with obsessive-compulsive symptoms. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 58, 51–69. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjc.12186
Soffer-Dudek, N. (2023). Obsessive-compulsive symptoms and dissociative experiences: Suggested underlying mechanisms and implications for science and practice. Frontiers in Psychology, 14, 1132800. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1132800
Soffer-Dudek, N., Lassri, D., Soffer-Dudek, N., & Shahar, G. (2015). Dissociative absorption: An empirically unique, clinically relevant, dissociative factor. Consciousness & Cognition, 36, 338–351. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2015.07.013
Soffer-Dudek, N., Shelef, L., Oz, I., Levkovsky, A., Erlich, I., & Gordon, S. (2017). Absorbed in sleep: Dissociative absorption as a predictor of sleepiness following sleep deprivation in two high-functioning samples. Consciousness and Cognition, 48, 161–170. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2016.11.009
Soffer-Dudek, N., & Somer, E. (2018). Trapped in a daydream: Daily elevations in maladaptive daydreaming are associated with daily psychopathological symptoms. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 9, 194. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2018.00194
Soffer-Dudek, N., & Somer, E. (2022). Maladaptive daydreaming is a dissociative disorder: Supporting evidence and theory. In M. J. Dorahy, S. N. Gold, & J. A. O’Neil (Eds.), Dissociation and the dissociative disorders: Past, present, future (2nd ed., pp. 547–559). Taylor & Francis.
Soffer-Dudek, N., Somer, E., Abu-Rayya, H. M., Metin, B., & Schimmenti, A. (2020). Different cultures, similar daydream addiction? An examination of the cross-cultural measurement equivalence of the maladaptive daydreaming scale. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 9(4), 1056–1067. https://doi.org/10.1556/2006.2020.00080
Soffer-Dudek, N., Wertheim, R., & Shahar, G. (2011). Lucid dreaming and resilience in the face of exposure to terrorism. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 24(1), 125–128. https://doi.org/10.1002/jts.20601
Somer, E. (2002). Maladaptive daydreaming: A qualitative inquiry. Journal of Contemporary Psychotherapy, 32(2), 195–210. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1020597026919
Somer, E. (2018). Maladaptive daydreaming: Ontological analysis, treatment rationale: A pilot case study. Frontiers in the Psychotherapy of Trauma and Dissociation, 2(1), 1–22. https://doi.org/10.46716/ftpd.2017.0006
Somer, E., Abu-Rayya, H. M., & Brenner, R. (2021). Childhood trauma and maladaptive daydreaming: Fantasy functions and themes in a multi-country sample. Journal of Trauma & Dissociation, 22(3), 288–303. https://doi.org/10.1080/15299732.2020.1809599
Somer, E., Cardeña, E., Catelan, R. F., & Soffer-Dudek, N. (2021). Reality shifting: Psychological features of an emergent online daydreaming culture. Current Psychology, 42, 11415–11427. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-021-02439-3
Somer, E., J., L., Jopp, D. S., & Bigelsen, J. (2016). Development and validation of the maladaptive daydreaming scale (MDS. Consciousness and Cognition, 39, 77–91. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2015.12.001
Somer, E., Soffer-Dudek, N., & Ross, C. A. (2017). The comorbidity of daydreaming disorder (maladaptive daydreaming. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 205(7), 525–530. https://doi.org/10.1097/NMD.0000000000000685
Somer, E., Soffer-Dudek, N., Ross, C. A., & Halpern, N. (2017). Maladaptive daydreaming: Proposed diagnostic criteria and their assessment with a structured clinical interview. Psychology of Consciousness: Theory, Research, and Practice, 4(2), 176–189. https://doi.org/10.1037/cns0000114
Sparrow, G. S. (2014). Analyzing chronic dream ego responses in co-creative dream analysis. International Journal of Dream Research, 7(1), 33–38. https://doi.org/10.11588/ijodr.2014.1.12128
Speckens, A. E. M., Hackmann, A., Ehlers, A., & Cuthbert, B. (2007). Imagery special issue: Intrusive images and memories of earlier adverse events in patients with obsessive compulsive disorder. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 38(4), 411–422. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbtep.2007.09.004
Spitzer, C., Barnow, S., Freyberger, H. J., & Grabe, H. J. (2007). Dissociation predicts symptom-related treatment outcome in short-term inpatient psychotherapy. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 41(8), 682–687. https://doi.org/10.1080/00048670701449146
Spoormaker, V. I., & Bout, J. van den. (2006). Lucid dreaming treatment for nightmares: A pilot study. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 75, 389–394. https://doi.org/10.1159/000095446
Spoormaker, V. I., Bout, J. van den, & Meijer, E. J. (2003). Lucid dreaming treatment for nightmares: A series of cases. Dreaming, 13, 181–186. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1025325529560
Sterpenich, V., Albouy, G., Boly, M., Vandewalle, G., Darsaud, A., Balteau, E., Dang-Vu, T. T., Desseilles, M., D’Argembeau, A., Gais, S., Rauchs, G., Schabus, M., Degueldre, C., Luxen, A., Collette, F., & Maquet, P. (2007). Sleep-related hippocampo-cortical interplay during emotional memory recollection. PLoS Biology, 5(11), 282. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.0050282
Sterpenich, V., Albouy, G., Darsaud, A., Schmidt, C., Vandewalle, G., Dang Vu, T. T., Desseilles, M., Phillips, C., Degueldre, C., Balteau, E., Collette, F., Luxen, A., & Maquet, P. (2009). Sleep promotes the neural reorganization of remote emotional memory. The Journal of Neuroscience, 29, 5143–5152. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0561-09.2009
Strachan, L. P., Hyett, M. P., & McEvoy, P. M. (2020). Imagery rescripting for anxiety disorders and obsessive-compulsive disorder: Recent advances and future directions. Current Psychiatry Reports, 22, 17. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11920-020-1139-4
Theodor-Katz, N., Somer, E., Maaravi Hesseg, R., & Soffer-Dudek, N. (2022). Could immersive daydreaming underlie a deficit in attention? The prevalence and characteristics of maladaptive daydreaming in individuals with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 78(11), 2309–2328. https://doi.org/10.1002/jclp.23355
Van der Kloet, D., Merckelbach, H., Giesbrecht, T., & Lynn, S. J. (2012). Fragmented sleep, fragmented mind: The role of sleep in dissociative symptoms. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 7(2), 159–175. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691612437597
Van Eeden, F. (1913). A study of dreams. Proceedings of the Society for Psychical Research, 26, 413–461. http://dreamscience.ca/en/documents/New%20content/lucid%20dreaming%20pdfs/vanEeden_PSPR_26_1-12_1913.pdf
Van Heughten – van der Kloet, D., Giesbrecht, T., & Merckelbach, H. (2015). Sleep loss increases dissociation and affects memory for emotional stimuli. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 47, 9–17. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbtep.2014.11.002
Van Rijn, E., Eichenlaub, J.-B., Lewis, P. A., Walker, M. P., Gaskell, M. G., Malinowski, J. E., & Blagrove, M. (2015). The dream-lag effect: Selective processing of personally significant events during rapid eye movement sleep, but not during slow wave sleep. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 122, 98–109. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nlm.2015.01.009
Voss, U., Holzmann, R., Tuin, I., & Hobson, J. A. (2009). Lucid dreaming: A state of consciousness with features of both waking and non-lucid dreaming. Sleep, 32(9), 1191–1200. https://doi.org/10.1093/sleep/32.9.1191
Wamsley, E., Donjacour, C. E., Scammell, T. E., Lammers, G. J., & Stickgold, R. (2014). Delusional confusion of dreaming and reality in narcolepsy. Sleep, 37(2), 419–422. https://doi.org/10.5665/sleep.3428
Watson, D. (2001). Dissociations of the night: Individual differences in sleep-related experiences and their relation to dissociation and schizotypy. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 110, 526–535. https://doi.org/10.1037//0021-843x.110.4.526
Wen, H., Soffer-Dudek, N., & Somer, E. (2022). Daily feelings and the affective valence of daydreams in maladaptive daydreaming: A longitudinal analysis. Psychology of Consciousness. https://doi.org/10.1037/cns0000293
West, M., & Somer, E. (2020). Empathy, emotion regulation and creativity in immersive daydreaming. Imagination, Cognition and Personality, 39(4), 358–373. https://doi.org/10.1177/0276236619864277
Windt, J., & Voss, U. (2018). Spontaneous thought, insight, and control in lucid dreams. In K. Christoff & K. C. R. Fox (Eds.), The oxford handbook of spontaneous thought: Mind-wandering, creativity, and dreaming (pp. 385–410). Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780190464745.013.26
Wolman, R. N., & Kozmová, M. (2007). Last night i had the strangest dream: Varieties of rational thought processes in dream reports. Consciousness and Cognition, 16, 838–849. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2006.09.009
Yu, C. K.-C. (2013). Obsessive-compulsive distress and its dynamic associations with schizotypy, borderline personality, and dreaming. Dreaming, 23(1), 46–63. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0030791
Zadra, A. L., & Pihl, R. O. (1997). Lucid dreaming as a treatment for recurrent nightmares. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 66, 50–55. https://doi.org/10.1159/000289106

  1. Different interpretations of continuity may be found in the literature cited here, a discussion of which is outside the scope of this work. Also, note that the degree to which dreams may be meaningful, in that they are related to waking life or waking thought, is a separate question from whether they have an adaptive function (Blagrove, 2011). The latter question is also outside the present scope.↩︎

  2. See (Dennett, 1976) for a lengthier discussion on this topic.↩︎

  3. Relatedly, at least in sleep onset dreams, a preliminary single-case study showed that linguistic versus perceptual dream intrusions into consciousness were associated with differential neural correlates (Noreika et al., 2015).↩︎

  4. A similar rapid circular dynamic of visual image formation as both affected by, and affecting, the temporal sequence was described regarding brief sleep onset dreams, but in the context of neuromuscular sensations, auditory elements, and various other exogenous stimuli (Nielsen, 2017).↩︎

  5. Notably, it has been shown that the direction of cortical information flow during waking visual imagery production is reversed compared to waking perception, using two different causality methods, supporting the classic distinction of top-down and bottom-up processing (Dentico et al., 2014). Perhaps future neuropsychological studies could provide evidence for similar top-down directionality during dreaming, to provide further support for the idea that dreaming is more like imagination than perception (Nir & Tononi, 2010).↩︎

  6. The notion that the absorption construct represents dissociation is a convention in the literature, but notably, “structural dissociation” theorists do not agree with that idea. This discussion is well beyond the scope of this work, but the interested reader is referred to Soffer-Dudek & Somer (2022) for an elaboration on the disagreement as well as an in-depth characterization of the dissociative nature of absorption.↩︎